Clark’s article, which explores how children fill gaps in
their vocabulary and acquire language through principles of semantic
transparency, productivity, and conventionality, reminded me of a poem I read
in high school, “in Just –” by E. E. Cummings, that embodies childhood. In the
poem, there are made-up words like “mud-luscious” and “puddle-wonderful” that,
despite their novelty, convey easily understood meanings by combining familiar
words. “Mud-luscious” and “puddle-wonderful” are like grown-up child-speak (as
in, Cummings, an adult with adult vocabulary, writes through a child’s
viewpoint), in the vein of Clark’s semantic transparency. We know what “mud” means and what “luscious”
means, and as adults tasked with combining the two, we might say something like
“lusciously muddy,” but Cummings’ curious “mud-luscious” invokes nuances of childlike
simplicity and wonder at a fresh world.
Some might say that when we become adults, we lose the novel,
unburdened perspectives of children because we become bound by all sorts of
conventions. As adults, we know words that are right on the dot, retrievable
labels for items in our world that children don’t know, so we don’t have to
innovate. Since children don’t know, they’ll come up with something – to
paraphrase Pablo Picasso, give them a blank vocabulary and they’ll fill it.
Like Clark mentions, a “break-man” could be someone who breaks things. Tea
could be “leaf water”; coffee could be “bean water.” Even if these terms are
not real or conventional words or phrases, they still work and show a different
way of thinking unique to children, like crawling through a space only big
enough for a child.
This ingenuity in children’s language acquisition is
fascinating, as the enormous feat of building up a world bit by bit,
continually adding, replacing, and substituting happens every day without much
thought. It’s absolutely mind-explosionfull.
No comments:
Post a Comment