Overall, both of these readings are
similar to one other because they are each related to linguistic implicature. Most
importantly, they both serve to highlight one of the limits of lexical
reasoning in children. Lexical reasoning may be limited when it is
context-independent, and this happens even when the capacity for inference is
already developed in a child.
For example, Experiment 1 in the
reading titled Ad-hoc scalar implicature
in adults and children by Stiller, Goodman, and Frank serves to demonstrate
precisely this: children have a developed ability to identify implicatures in
language and they demonstrate it when given the visual context of the
situation; however, they have trouble dealing with the “lexical items” (the
words themselves) and their scales. The same limit to lexical reasoning is
evident in the reading titled Accessing
the unsaid: The role of scalar alternatives in children’s pragmatic inference by
Barner, Brooks, and Bale. In this reading, it is demonstrated that children
have a much harder time reasoning with the word “only” when contextual
alternatives are not provided than when they are. For instance, when no context
was provided, children would continuously misjudge the number of animals that
were sleeping (as in the 3-item case).
Both of these examples demonstrate
that children routinely fail in detecting linguistic implicatures when dealing
with context-independent situations. And in both cases, adults outperformed
children. However, in the same way in which certain lexical processes are
limited in children as compared to adults, I wonder whether for other
situations, adults themselves have a limited ability to make sense of words
when they’re not provided with contextual information or visual examples. Children
performed better when they were provided with the context that allowed them to
more easily visualize or make sense of the situation at hand. Having said this,
are there certain situations in which adults could significantly improve their
lexical reasoning when provided with context or visual cues?
There are different cases that come to mind when I think of potential examples. One of them is a double or even triple negative. We might find ourselves confused if we hear someone say: “We can’t not go to the park”. However, it wouldn’t be tough to understand a visual that shows a person simply walking to the park, for that is precisely what the statement implies – “We can go to the park”. These and other cases demonstrate that in some situations, our lexical reasoning could appear compromised even when we understand the underlying processes that are taking place. I also wonder whether biases are in some way related to this – when we’re unconsciously biased, maybe we’re having trouble making sense of the lexical situation, even though, if we stop, think, and visualize, we might understand the underlying processes that are going on.
After your hit linguistics seminal paper "Heuristics in Language", I was excitedaf to read what you had to share this week, J-Rendyn. I liked how you talked about the case of multiple negatives. I can't not agree with you that it is easy to deduce "we can't not go to the park" means "we can go to the park", and I see the point you make when taking into account other instances of multiple negatives, such as "we never don't not have a problem", which obviously means "we never have a problem". Since you speak about how visuals can improve understanding, what visual do you think could make it easier to understand this previous statement? An empty set of problems, perhaps?
ReplyDelete