The Lupyan article was comprised of
4 different experiments that analyzed how our minds deal with the categorization
of objects. The paper began with the interesting realization that our brains
may not be able to create abstract images, something that initially took some
thought to come to terms with. The idea that whenever we hear or see a word and
picture it in our head, we are possibly just flashing an image of one
particular instance of that object, reminded me of dreaming. We know so little
about how dreams occur or why we experience what we do, but it seems publically
accepted knowledge that dreams are comprised of real events, people, emotions,
that have occurred throughout our recent days. Similarly, every face that we
see in dreams is recognizable and can be linked to a face we saw when awake but
we can never create a completely new face during a dream. This goes to support how
tough true abstraction is for our brain to grasp, if we even can.
Anyways, the Lupyan article focused
mainly on how descriptions of shapes can alter our perception of them in our
minds. In the experiments described, subjects were asked to draw either a triangle
or a three sided figure to test whether any differences in outcomes would
arise. And they did. When asked to draw a triangle, the shape that was drawn is
the more ‘typical’ picture of a triangle, either equilateral or isosceles. However,
when asked to draw the three sided shape, the shapes drawn moved further away
from the ‘typical’ triangle and showed more variance. The ultimate take away
from the article is the information it provided us on how representations occur
in our brains: that our brains categorize things into “idealized perceptual
states” (21) which can be elicited by language. This shows the important and
influence that language can have on how we perceive the world inside of our
brains.
The Rickford article was a complete
180 from the Lupyan article but was still interesting to read. Rickford’s begins
by explaining the important influence and effect the African American speech
community has had over the past 30 years on American sociolinguistics. He then goes on to contrast this by explaining
the contributions sociolinguistics have provided the African American speech
community which are thoroughly lacking. He describes several of the issues at
hand: how African Americans are underrepresented in the linguistic community, the
misrepresentations of African Americans community within writings have caused
an “impression that black speech is the lingo of criminals” (171), and then
many more social and legal reasons which have negatively affected the African
American communities. Rickford then provides a call to action, giving the
reader several possible solutions to some of the problems he raised. He names
two main activities in which we can get involved in order to begin the process
of giving back. The first focuses on scholarly/academic changes such as
training and employing more African American linguists, making legal
contributions to employment issues, and just getting involved in classrooms
with African American students. The second activity is to mainly be a positive
individual and role model within one’s community by tutoring, helping out in
the soup kitchens, initiating book drives and so on. Rickford’s main goal of
the paper is to explain the one sided relationship between sociolinguistics and
the African American speech community, and then provide us with the methods we can
use to go about helping to bridge the gap that is currently limiting the
relationship between American sociolinguistics and the African American speech
community.
On the subject of language affecting our perceptions, I found one particular study really interesting (Loftus and Palmer 1974). The study consisted of two experiments. In the first, participants were shown seven videos of a particular car accident, and after the videos they were asked several specific questions including, "How fast were the cars going when they smashed/bumped/hit/contacted each other?" The researchers found that the speed estimated varied with the verb in the question; the participants asked the "smashed" variant gave the highest average estimate of 40.8 mph, whereas those who were asked the "contacted" variant gave the lowest average answer of 31.8 mph. The second study attempted to study a similar phenomenon. The participants were shown a one-minbute film of a car driving through the countryside followed by an accident. Afterwards the students were asked one of three questions: 1) "How fast were the cars going when they hit each other?" 2) "How fast were the cars going when they smashed each other?" or 3) No question at all. One week later, the participants were asked to report whether they had seen any broken glass. Not surprisingly, participants who were asked whether the cars had smashed were the most likely to report having seen broken glass.
ReplyDelete