In his paper The
paradox of the universal triangle, Gary Lupyan uses simple triangle-drawing
experiments to investigate the way our brains represent concepts and link them
to words. This question is an ongoing
debate in science and philosophy, and one theory is that our brains store each
concept as a single entity that can be evoked by any description of it. For example, the concept of a triangle could
be called up by ‘triangle,’ ‘three-sided polygon,’ or any other correct
description. If this were the case, then
we would expect that asking people to draw triangles or three-sided polygons
would yield the same results. Lupyan performed
this experiment, found that prompt wording actually made a big difference –
when people were specifically asked to draw ‘triangles’, they drew much more
standard figures (almost all subjects drew upwards-pointing equilateral
triangles) than when asked to draw ‘three-sided polygons’. This supports the idea that labels like ‘triangle’
evoke specific perceptual states. According
to this theory, called “simulation,” labels allow us to aggregate many
different experiences with specific categories and thereby learn the common
characteristics that define the category.
In the case of a triangle, most of these experiences involve
equilateral, upward-pointing triangles, so when people access their internal
representation of a triangle, that specific standard triangle gets
recreated. Asking people to draw a ‘three
sided figure’ activates a different set of experiences and representations, leading
to greater variability in the resulting figures. In addition, Lupyan found that standard-looking
triangles were actually recognized faster than atypical ones, reinforcing the
idea that people are comparing visual input with an internal triangle
representation that reflects the most common types of triangles.
The connection between classification and language is one of
the most interesting parts of Lupyan’s article.
He writes that “language impairments such as aphasia impair
categorization in nonverbal contexts” (3). I would not have expected language and
classification to be so deeply related. If
language is necessary for categorization, then does this prevent animals from
effectively classifying objects? Could the
need for categorization have driven development of human language?
In Unequal
partnership: Sociolinguistics and the African American speech community,
John Rickford discusses how the study of AAVE has advanced linguistics and improved
our understanding of the process of grammatical change. Sadly, AAVE continues to be perceived as ‘less
correct’ than SE, leading to discrimination in job interviews, court cases, and
other situations. Rickford discusses multiple
ways that linguistics researchers and linguistics programs can help. For example, he talks about the Industrial
Language Training (ILT) program used in the UK, which worked with employers to
help modify their negative attitudes towards people with different accents or
dialects. Another key area where
linguistics research can be applied is childhood education. Linguistics experiments have shown that
reading instruction where AAVE-speaking students read the same stories in both
AAVE and SE can be far more successful than standard lessons. In one case, the resulting student score
increase corresponded to 6.2 months of instruction in just four months. However, these programs are often
misunderstood by the public and face harsh criticism, so they are not widely
used. Hopefully linguistics research
will continue to influence public education and lead to better learning
outcomes.
No comments:
Post a Comment