The Gussenhoven and Kenstowicz
papers worked well together in providing a deeper introduction into speech and
speech perception. Gussenhoven focused mainly on the physical aspects behind
the production of speech. He delves into what causes the creation of sound, describing
the role that the lungs have on maintaining and generating changes in air
pressure, how the opening and closing of the glottis produces a large number of
consonants, and how the vibrating of our vocal cords is what drives our voices.
All of these can be manipulated to create
three unique types of phonation: whisper, breathy, and creaky voice. Whispering
occurs when the vocal cords are brought together to increase friction when air
flows. The breathy voice is produced when one speaks but the glottis remains
open throughout, causing air to constantly flow out of the body. And lastly,
the creaky voice results from an extreme tightening of the vocal cords,
allowing each opening/closing to sound as if it was an individual event.
The Kenstowicz paper disregards the
anatomical aspects of speech and focuses more on how humans understand words
and sentences once they are actually spoken. He explains that many letters have
several different pronunciations given the word they are being used in, which forces
the speaker to follow certain rules of speech. For example, when analyzing the
[t] sound, it can phonetically be misinterpreted for the [D] sound. When speaking,
‘tends’ and ‘tents’ are pronounced so similarly that in fluid conversation, it
would be hard to distinguish between the two. Kenstowicz then explains that
this leads to unspoken rules within each language that should be followed in
order to be understood correctly.
The interesting thing about reading
these two paper is, especially in the Kenstowicz paper, that I read most of it
aloud, sounding out the different words to see whether the differences he was referring
to where actually audible. They were, and it immediately reminded me of little children
reading their picture books following their finger word by word as they
pronounce them slowly. So many times, as younger children are beginning to
learn to read, they are taught that each letter has a sound and in order to read
words, you put each letter’s sound together. However, they quickly find out
that the sum of the parts does not make up the whole; there is so much more to
the words than just the individual sounds crammed together. Words like ‘knight’,
‘lamb’, ‘daughter’, ‘half’ were constant struggles for the kids. Why not just
write ‘lamb’ without the ‘b’? English written and oral language have massive
discrepancies making them more complicated than one would initially imagine. But
why is that so? Do we say ‘tends’ and ‘tents’ similarly just because we are too
lazy to properly articulate the difference, and if so, why have we allowed laziness
to convolute our methods of communication?
No comments:
Post a Comment