The
readings for this week continued the trend of building upon the ideas of the
prior week. The Atkins-Levin reading discussed semantics and the accuracy of
dictionaries in presenting words. The current electronic corpus available to us
provides an overwhelming amount of linguistic data. How do we select what to
put in the dictionary when words are used in so many ways? More importantly,
how do we describe words in a way that makes sense? This prompted a discussion
of near-synonyms. The example given was that of the shake verb family,
including quake, quiver, shake, shiver, shudder, tremble, and vibrate.
Atkins-Levin explored the notion that the transitive and intransitive nature of
verbs is idiosyncratic. These idiosyncrasies can make the verbs difficult to
define.
One statement in particular
interested me. The sentence “No dictionary of a living tongue can ever be
perfect” encapsulated the main issue of the article. Words surprise us. The
rules we create to define them and their semantics are so often broken. The
internal or external causation of the shake verb family can perhaps determine
their idiosyncratic behavior. But, as stated by the article, each dictionary
fails at some points in describing with precision the lexicon available. It
also raises the question of how to define words at all. Every word that we
write down potentially contains its own set of irregularities. This especially
appeared in the various dictionary definitions of the shake verb family. Many
of the verbs utilized other verbs in the same family for their definitions,
introducing these idiosyncrasies into the very definitions of the verbs
themselves.
This idea of a varied, diverse, and
difficult to describe nature of language also appears in the writings of
Haspelmath. This piece had both a morphological and a syntactical focus. For
instance, even when dictionaries define words, as discussed by Atkins-Levin,
they do not include every sequence of letters. The dictionary definitions are
lexemes, while the words that are actually pronounced and used in text are
word-forms. Inflection refers to the relationship between word-forms of a
lexeme. Tree diagrams can be helpful in showing the hierarchy of morphology.
Two interesting examples I found about the construction of words are lipstick and child support. These words are not lipsstick and children
support. Thus, some morphological rule governs the combinations of compound
words in a way that doesn’t directly state their meaning.
Building upon the writings of Haspelmath,
Slobin explored the different ways we have of describing different scenarios
based on the type of language we speak. The article looked at verb-framed
languages and satellite-framed languages. It dealt with the story of a frog and
how speakers choose to describe events. How is the manner of motion expressed
and how is the path of motion expressed? For me, the discussion of English was
most interesting because Slobin found that English speakers tend to embellish
even further. This is intriguing because English already has so many more words
than other languages yet speakers still augment with more adjectives. Finally,
Slobin concludes that much of the diversity and irregularity we see in
language, as discussed by Haspelmath and Atkins-Levin, derives from habitual
patterns shaped by ease of linguistic form and cultural and aesthetic aims.
I was also fascinated by the concept expressed by Atkins-Levin that dictionaries aren't getting bigger, that more stuff is getting left out every year. What do you think would be the best way to try to make a "perfect" dictionary of a "living tongue"? Atkins and Levin seem to be detailing what still needs to be done in the field, but how would a dictionary use their findings without becoming so long as to be burdensome?
ReplyDelete